English phonology


Like many other languages, English has wide variation in pronunciation, both historically and from dialect to dialect. In general, however, the regional dialects of English share a largely similar phonological system. Among other things, most dialects have vowel reduction in unstressed syllables and a complex set of phonological features that distinguish fortis and lenis consonants.
Phonological analysis of English often concentrates on or uses, as a reference point, one or more of the prestige or standard accents, such as Received Pronunciation for England, General American for the United States, and General Australian for Australia. Nevertheless, many other dialects of English are spoken, which have developed independently from these standardized accents, particularly regional dialects. Information about these standardized accents functions only as a limited guide to all of English phonology, which one can later expand upon once one becomes more familiar with some of the many other dialects of English that are spoken.

Phonemes

A phoneme of a language or dialect is an abstraction of a speech sound or of a group of different sounds which are all perceived to have the same function by speakers of that particular language or dialect. For example, the English word through consists of three phonemes: the initial "th" sound, the "r" sound, and a vowel sound. The phonemes in this and many other English words do not always correspond directly to the letters used to spell them.
The number and distribution of phonemes in English vary from dialect to dialect, and also depend on the interpretation of the individual researcher. The number of consonant phonemes is generally put at 24. The number of vowels is subject to greater variation; in the system presented on this page there are 20–25 vowel phonemes in Received Pronunciation, 14–16 in General American and 19–20 in Australian English. The pronunciation keys used in dictionaries generally contain a slightly greater number of symbols than this, to take account of certain sounds used in foreign words and certain noticeable distinctions that may not be—strictly speaking—phonemic.

[|Consonants]

The following table shows the 24 consonant phonemes found in most dialects of English, plus, whose distribution is more limited. Fortis consonants are always voiceless, aspirated in syllable onset, and sometimes also glottalized to an extent in syllable coda, while lenis consonants are always unaspirated and un-glottalized, and generally partially or fully voiced. The alveolars are usually apical, i.e. pronounced with the tip of the tongue touching or approaching the roof of the mouth, though some speakers produce them laminally, i.e. with the blade of the tongue.

Consonant examples

The following table shows typical examples of the occurrence of the above consonant phonemes in words.

Sonorants

In most dialects, the fortis stops and affricate have various different allophones, and are distinguished from the lenis stops and affricate by several phonetic features.
English has a particularly large number of vowel phonemes, and in addition the vowels of English differ considerably between dialects. Because of this, corresponding vowels may be transcribed with various symbols depending on the dialect under consideration. When considering English as a whole, lexical sets are often used, each named by a word containing the vowel or vowels in question. For example, the set consists of words which, like lot, have in Received Pronunciation and in General American. The " vowel" then refers to the vowel that appears in those words in whichever dialect is being considered, or to a diaphoneme, which represents this interdialectal correspondence. A commonly used system of lexical sets, devised by John C. Wells, is presented below; for each set, the corresponding phonemes are given for RP and General American, using the notation that will be used on this page.







,
,




For a table that shows the pronunciations of these vowels in a wider range of English dialects, see IPA chart for English dialects.
The following tables show the vowel phonemes of three standard varieties of English. The notation system used here for Received Pronunciation is fairly standard; the others less so. The feature descriptions given here are abstracted somewhat; the actual pronunciations of these vowels are somewhat more accurately conveyed by the IPA symbols used.






The differences between these tables can be explained as follows:
Other points to be noted are these:
Listed here are some of the significant cases of allophony of vowels found within standard English dialects.
s in English may contain almost any vowel, but in practice vowels in stressed and unstressed syllables tend to use different inventories of phonemes. In particular, long vowels are used less often in unstressed syllables than stressed syllables. Additionally there are certain sounds—characterized by central position and weakness—that are particularly often found as the nuclei of unstressed syllables. These include:
Vowel reduction in unstressed syllables is a significant feature of English. Syllables of the types listed above often correspond to a syllable containing a different vowel used in other forms of the same morpheme where that syllable is stressed. For example, the first o in photograph, being stressed, is pronounced with the vowel, but in photography, where it is unstressed, it is reduced to schwa. Also, certain common words are pronounced with a schwa when they are unstressed, although they have different vowels when they are in a stressed position.
Some unstressed syllables, however, retain full vowels, i.e. vowels other than those listed above. Examples are the in ambition and the in finite. Some phonologists regard such syllables as not being fully unstressed ; some dictionaries have marked such syllables as having secondary stress. However linguists such as Ladefoged and regard this as a difference purely of vowel quality and not of stress, and thus argue that vowel reduction itself is phonemic in English. Examples of words where vowel reduction seems to be distinctive for some speakers include chickaree vs. chicory, and Pharaoh vs. farrow.

Lexical stress

is phonemic in English. For example, the noun increase and the verb increase are distinguished by the positioning of the stress on the first syllable in the former, and on the second syllable in the latter. Stressed syllables in English are louder than non-stressed syllables, as well as being longer and having a higher pitch.
In traditional approaches, in any English word consisting of more than one syllable, each syllable is ascribed one of three degrees of stress: primary, secondary or unstressed. Ordinarily, in each such word there will be exactly one syllable with primary stress, possibly one syllable having secondary stress, and the remainder are unstressed. For example, the word amazing has primary stress on the second syllable, while the first and third syllables are unstressed, whereas the word organization has primary stress on the fourth syllable, secondary stress on the first, and the second, third and fifth unstressed. This is often shown in pronunciation keys using the IPA symbols for primary and secondary stress, placed before the syllables to which they apply. The two words just given may therefore be represented as and.
Some analysts identify an additional level of stress. This is generally ascribed to syllables that are pronounced with less force than those with secondary stress, but nonetheless contain a "full" or "unreduced" vowel. Hence the third syllable of organization, if pronounced with as shown above, might be said to have tertiary stress.
In some analyses, then, the concept of lexical stress may become conflated with that of vowel reduction. An approach which attempts to separate these two is provided by Peter Ladefoged, who states that it is possible to describe English with only one degree of stress, as long as unstressed syllables are phonemically distinguished for vowel reduction. In this approach, the distinction between primary and secondary stress is regarded as a phonetic or prosodic detail rather than a phonemic feature – primary stress is seen as an example of the predictable "tonic" stress that falls on the final stressed syllable of a prosodic unit. For more details of this analysis, see Stress and vowel reduction in English.
For stress as a prosodic feature, see below.

Phonotactics

is the study of the sequences of phonemes that occur in languages and the sound structures that they form. In this study it is usual to represent consonants in general with the letter C and vowels with the letter V, so that a syllable such as 'be' is described as having CV structure. The IPA symbol used to show a division between syllables is the dot. Syllabification is the process of dividing continuous speech into discrete syllables, a process in which the position of a syllable division is not always easy to decide upon.
Most languages of the world syllabify and sequences as and or, with consonants preferentially acting as the onset of a syllable containing the following vowel. According to one view, English is unusual in this regard, in that stressed syllables attract following consonants, so that and syllabify as and, as long as the consonant cluster is a possible syllable coda; in addition, preferentially syllabifies with the preceding vowel even when both syllables are unstressed, so that occurs as. This is the analysis used in the Longman Pronunciation Dictionary. However, this view is not widely accepted, as explained in the following section.

Syllable structure

The syllable structure in English is 3V5, with a near maximal example being strengths. From the phonetic point of view, the analysis of syllable structures is a complex task: because of widespread occurrences of articulatory overlap, English speakers rarely produce an audible release of individual consonants in consonant clusters. This coarticulation can lead to articulatory gestures that seem very much like deletions or complete assimilations. For example, hundred pounds may sound like and jumped back may sound like, but X-ray and electropalatographic studies demonstrate that inaudible and possibly weakened contacts or lingual gestures may still be made. Thus the second in hundred pounds does not entirely assimilate to a labial place of articulation, rather the labial gesture co-occurs with the alveolar one; the "missing" in jumped back may still be articulated, though not heard.
Division into syllables is a difficult area, and different theories have been proposed. A widely accepted approach is the maximal onset principle: this states that, subject to certain constraints, any consonants in between vowels should be assigned to the following syllable. Thus the word leaving should be divided rather than *, and hasty is rather than * or *. However, when such a division results in an onset cluster which is not allowed in English, the division must respect this. Thus if the word extra were divided * the resulting onset of the second syllable would be, a cluster which does not occur initially in English. The division is therefore preferred. If assigning a consonant or consonants to the following syllable would result in the preceding syllable ending in an unreduced short vowel, this is avoided. Thus the word comma should be divided and not *, even though the latter division gives the maximal onset to the following syllable.
In some cases, no solution is completely satisfactory: for example, in British English the word hurry could be divided or, but the former would result in an analysis with a syllable-final while the latter would result in a syllable final . Some phonologists have suggested a compromise analysis where the consonant in the middle belongs to both syllables, and is described as ambisyllabic. In this way, it is possible to suggest an analysis of hurry which comprises the syllables and, the medial being ambisyllabic.
Where the division coincides with a word boundary, or the boundary between elements of a compound word, it is not usual in the case of dictionaries to insist on the maximal onset principle in a way that divides words in a counter-intuitive way; thus the word hardware would be divided by the M.O.P., but dictionaries prefer the division.
In the approach used by the Longman Pronunciation Dictionary, Wells claims that consonants syllabify with the preceding rather than following vowel when the preceding vowel is the nucleus of a more salient syllable, with stressed syllables being the most salient, reduced syllables the least, and full unstressed vowels intermediate. But there are lexical differences as well, frequently but not exclusively with compound words. For example, in dolphin and selfish, Wells argues that the stressed syllable ends in, but in shellfish, the belongs with the following syllable: →, but →, where the is a little longer and the is not reduced. Similarly, in toe-strap Wells argues that the second is a full plosive, as usual in syllable onset, whereas in toast-rack the second is in many dialects reduced to the unreleased allophone it takes in syllable codas, or even elided: → ; likewise nitrate → with a voiceless , vs night-rate → with a voiced. Cues of syllable boundaries include aspiration of syllable onsets and flapping of coda , epenthetic stops like in syllable codas, and r-colored vowels when the is in the coda vs. labialization when it is in the onset .

Onset

The following can occur as the onset:
All single consonant phonemes except genre
Stop plus approximant other than :
,,,,,,,,,,,,,,
play, blood, clean, glove, prize, bring, tree, dream, crowd, green, twin, dwarf, Guam, quick, puissance
Voiceless fricative or plus approximant other than :
,,,,,,,,,
floor, sleep, thlipsis, friend, three, shrimp, what, swing, thwart, reservoir
Consonant plus :
,,,,,,,,,,,,,,
pure, beautiful, tube, during, cute, argue, music, new, few, view, thew, suit, Zeus, huge, lurid
plus voiceless stop:
,,
speak, stop, skill
plus nasal other than :
,
smile, snow
plus voiceless fricative:
,
sphere, sthenic
plus voiceless stop plus approximant:
,,,,,,,,,
split, sclera, spring, street, scream, square, smew, spew, student, skewer
plus voiceless fricative plus approximant:
sphragistics

Notes:
Other onsets
Certain English onsets appear only in contractions: e.g. , and or . Some, such as , , or , can occur in interjections. An archaic voiceless fricative plus nasal exists, , as does an archaic .
Several additional onsets occur in loan words such as , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , and .
Some clusters of this type can be converted to regular English phonotactics by simplifying the cluster: e.g. , , , , , , , , and .
Others can be replaced by native clusters differing only in voice: , and .

Nucleus

The following can occur as the nucleus:
Most of the following except those that end with,,,, or can be extended with or representing the morpheme -s/-z. Similarly, most of the following except those that end with or can be extended with or representing the morpheme -t/-d.
argues that a variety of syllable codas are possible in English, even in words like entry and sundry, with being treated as affricates along the lines of. He argues that the traditional assumption that pre-vocalic consonants form a syllable with the following vowel is due to the influence of languages like French and Latin, where syllable structure is CVC.CVC regardless of stress placement. Disregarding such contentious cases, which do not occur at the ends of words, the following sequences can occur as the coda:
The single consonant phonemes except,, and, in non-rhotic varieties,
Lateral approximant plus stop or affricate:,,,,,, help, bulb, belt, hold, belch, indulge, milk
In rhotic varieties, plus stop or affricate:,,,,,,, harp, orb, fort, beard, arch, large, mark, morgue
Lateral approximant + fricative:,,,,, golf, solve, wealth, else, bells, Welsh
In rhotic varieties, + fricative:,,,,, dwarf, carve, north, force, Mars, marsh
Lateral approximant + nasal:, film, kiln
In rhotic varieties, + nasal or lateral:,, arm, born, snarl
Nasal + homorganic stop or affricate:,,,,, jump, tent, end, lunch, lounge, pink
Nasal + fricative:,,,,, in some varietiestriumph, warmth, month, prince, bronze, length
Voiceless fricative plus voiceless stop:,,, left, crisp, lost, ask
Two voiceless fricatives: fifth
Two voiceless stops:, opt, act
Stop plus voiceless fricative:,,,,, depth, lapse, eighth, klutz, width, box
Lateral approximant + two consonants:,,,,,, sculpt, alps, twelfth, waltz, whilst, mulct, calx
In rhotic varieties, + two consonants:,,,,, warmth, excerpt, corpse, quartz, horst, infarct
Nasal + homorganic stop + stop or fricative:,,, ,, in some varietiesprompt, glimpse, thousandth, distinct, jinx, length
Three obstruents:, sixth, next

For some speakers, a fricative before is elided so that these never appear phonetically: becomes, becomes, becomes.

Syllable-level patterns

The prosodic features of English – stress, rhythm, and intonation – can be described as follows.

Prosodic stress

Prosodic stress is extra stress given to words or syllables when they appear in certain positions in an utterance, or when they receive special emphasis.
According to Ladefoged's analysis, English normally has prosodic stress on the final stressed syllable in an intonation unit. This is said to be the origin of the distinction traditionally made at the lexical level between primary and secondary stress: when a word like admiration is spoken in isolation, or at the end of a sentence, the syllable ra is pronounced with greater force than the syllable ad, although when the word is not pronounced with this final intonation there may be no difference between the levels of stress of these two syllables.
Prosodic stress can shift for various pragmatic functions, such as focus or contrast. For instance, in the dialogue Is it brunch tomorrow? No, it's dinner tomorrow, the extra stress shifts from the last stressed syllable of the sentence, tomorrow, to the last stressed syllable of the emphasized word, dinner.
Grammatical function words are usually prosodically unstressed, although they can acquire stress when emphasized. Many English function words have distinct strong and weak pronunciations; for example, the word a in the last example is pronounced, while the more common unstressed a is pronounced. See Weak and strong forms in English.

Rhythm

English is claimed to be a stress-timed language. That is, stressed syllables tend to appear with a more or less regular rhythm, while non-stressed syllables are shortened to accommodate this. For example, in the sentence One make of car is better than another, the syllables one, make, car, bett- and ' will be stressed and relatively long, while the other syllables will be considerably shorter. The theory of stress-timing predicts that each of the three unstressed syllables in between bett- and ' will be shorter than the syllable of between make and car, because three syllables must fit into the same amount of time as that available for of. However, it should not be assumed that all varieties of English are stress-timed in this way. The English spoken in the West Indies, in Africa and in India are probably better characterized as syllable-timed, though the lack of an agreed scientific test for categorizing an accent or language as stress-timed or syllable-timed may lead one to doubt the value of such a characterization.

Intonation

Phonological contrasts in intonation can be said to be found in three different and independent domains. In the work of Halliday the following names are proposed:
These terms have been used in more recent work, though they have been criticized for being difficult to remember. American systems such as ToBI also identify contrasts involving boundaries between intonation phrases, placement of pitch accent, and choice of tone or tones associated with the pitch accent.
Example of phonological contrast involving placement of intonation unit boundaries :
Example of phonological contrast involving placement of tonic syllable :
Example of phonological contrast involving choice of tone
There is typically a contrast involving tone between wh-questions and yes/no questions, the former having a falling tone and the latter a rising tone, though studies of spontaneous speech have shown frequent exceptions to this rule. Tag questions asking for information are said to carry rising tones while those asking for confirmation have falling tone.

History of English pronunciation

The pronunciation system of English has undergone many changes throughout the history of the language, from the phonological system of Old English, to that of Middle English, through to that of the present day. Variation between dialects has always been significant. Former pronunciations of many words are reflected in their spellings, as English orthography has generally not kept pace with phonological changes since the Middle English period.
The English consonant system has been relatively stable over time, although a number of significant changes have occurred. Examples include the loss of the and sounds still reflected by the in words like night and taught, and the splitting of voiced and voiceless allophones of fricatives into separate phonemes. There have also been many changes in consonant clusters, mostly reductions, for instance those that produced the usual modern pronunciations of such letter combinations as, and English wh|.
The development of vowels has been much more complex. One of the most notable series of changes is that known as the Great Vowel Shift, which began around the late 14th century. Here the and in words like price and mouth became diphthongized, and other long vowels became higher: became , became and later , became , and became and later . These shifts are responsible for the modern pronunciations of many written vowel combinations, including those involving a silent final.
Many other changes in vowels have taken place over the centuries. These various changes mean that many words that formerly rhymed no longer do. For example, in Shakespeare's time, following the Great Vowel Shift, food, good and blood all had the vowel, but in modern pronunciation good has been shortened to, while blood has been shortened and lowered to in most accents. In other cases, words that were formerly distinct have come to be pronounced the same – examples of such mergers include meet–meat, pane–pain and toe–tow.

Citations