Small clause
In linguistics, a small clause consists of a subject and its predicate, but lacks an overt expression of tense. Small clauses have the semantic subject-predicate characteristics of a clause, and have some, but not all, the properties of a constituent. Structural analyses of small clauses vary according to whether a flat or layered analysis is pursued. The small clause is related to the phenomena of raising-to-object, exceptional case-marking, accusativus cum infinitivo, and object control.
History
Williams (1975, 1980)
The term "small clause" was coined by Edwin Williams in 1975, who specifically looked at "reduced relatives, adverbial modifier phrases, and gerundive phrases". The following three examples are treated in Williams' 1975 paper as"small clauses", as cited in Balazs 2012. However, not all linguists do consider these to be small clauses according to the term's modern definition.- The man is Norton's best friend.
- John decided to leave, .
- infuriates me.
Stowell (1981)
Timothy Stowell in 1981 proposed that the small clause is indeed a constituent, and proposed a structure for small clauses using X-bar theory. Stowell proposes that the subject is defined as an NP occurring in a specifier position, that case is assigned in the specifier position, and that not all categories have subjects. His analysis explains why case-marked subjects cannot occur in infinitival clauses, although NPs can be projected up to an infinitival clause's specifier position. Stowell considers the following examples to be small clauses and constituents.- I consider
- I expect
- We feared
- I saw
Small clause behaviour
A small clause divides into two constituents: the subject and its predicate. While small clauses occur cross-linguistically, different languages have different restrictions on what can and cannot be a well-formed small clause. Criteria for identifying a small clause include:- absence of tense-marking on the predicate
- possibility of negating the small clause predicate
- selectional restrictions imposed by the matrix verb that introduces the small clause
- constituency tests
Absence of tense-marking
There are two schools of thought regarding NP VP constructions. Some linguists believe that a small clause characteristically lacks a verb, while others believe that a small clause may have a verb but lacks inflected tense. The following three examples involve NP AP, NP DP, and NP PP constructions. These small clauses all lack verbs:
- I consider
- I consider
- I consider
- I consider Mary to be smart
- I consider Mary to be my best friend
- I consider Mary to be out of her mind
The second theory proposes that small clauses lack inflected tense but can have a bare infinitival verb. Under this theory, NP VP constructions are allowed. The following examples contrast small clauses with non-finite verbs with main clauses with finite verbs.
- They think they are ready to leave.
- *They think they are ready left.
- They think they must leave.
Selectional restrictions
Selected by Matrix Verb
Small clauses satisfy selectional requirements of the verb in the main clause in order to be grammatical.The argument structure of verbs is satisfied with small clause constructions. The following two examples show how the argument structure of the verb "consider" affects what predicate can be in the small clause.
- I consider Mr. Nyman a genius.
- *I consider Mr. Nyman in my shed.
However, this theory of selectional requirement is also disputed, as substitution of different small clauses can create grammatical readings.
- I consider the team in no fit state to play.
- *I consider my friends on the roof.
The matrix verb's selection of case also supports the theory that the matrix verb's selectional requirements affect small clause licensing.
The verb "consider" marks accusative case on the small clause's subject NP. This conclusion is supported by pronoun-substitution.
- I consider Natasha a visionary.
- I consider her a visionary.
- *I consider she a visionary.
Semantically Determined
Small clauses' grammaticality judgments are affected by their semantic value.The following examples show how semantic selection also affects predication of a small clause.
- *The doctor considers that patient dead tomorrow.
- Our pilot considers that island off our route.
- *I consider John off my ship.
- As soon as he sets foot on the gangplank, I'll consider John off my ship.
Negation
- I consider Rome not a good choice.
- *I consider Rome might not a good choice.
Constituency tests
There are a number of considerations that support or refute the one or the other analysis. The layered analysis, which, again, views the small clause as a constituent, is supported by the basic insight that the small clause functions as a single semantic unit, i.e. as a clause consisting of a subject and a predicate.Coordination
Coordination assumes only constituents of a like type can be joined with a conjunction. Small clauses can be coordinated with conjunctions, which suggests they are constituents of a like type.The example below shows small clause coordination for NP AP phrases, and NP NP/DP phrases respectively.
- He considers and .
- She considers and .
Subject of a small clause
Concerning small clauses in subject position, see Culicover, Haegeman and Guéron.
Complement of 'with'
Most theories of syntax judge subjects to be single constituents, hence the small clauses Bill behind the wheel and Sam drunk here should each be construed as one constituent. Further, small clauses can appear as the complement of with, e.g.:These data are also easier to accommodate if the small clause is a constituent.
Movement
One can argue, however, that small clauses in subject position and as the complement of with are fundamentally different from small clauses in object position. The data further above have the small clause following the matrix verb, whereby the subject of the small clause is also the object of the matrix clause. In such cases, the matrix verb appears to be subcategorizing for its object noun, which then functions as the subject of the small clause. In this regard, there are a number of observations suggesting that the object/subject noun phrase is a direct dependent of the matrix verb, which means the flat structure is correct: the small clause generally does not behave as a single constituent with respect to constituency tests; the object becomes the subject of the corresponding passive sentence; and when the object is a reflexive pronoun, it is coindexed with the matrix subject:These data are consistent with the flat analysis of small clauses. The object of the matrix clause plays a dual role insofar as it is also the subject of the embedded predicate.
Counter-Arguments
Small clauses' constituency status is not agreed upon by linguists. Some linguists argue that small clauses do not form a constituent, but rather forms a noun phrase.One argument is that NP AP small clauses cannot occur in the subject position without modification. However, these NP AP small clauses can occur after the verb without modification, such as in example 1.
- * is a common problem in libraries.
- is a common problem in libraries.
- Louis gave and .
- They sponged the water up.
Structural analyses
Broadly speaking, there are three competing analyses of the structure of small clauses, flat, layered, and X-bar theory. The flat analysis judges the subject and predicate of the small clause to be sister constituents, whereas the layered analysis and X-bar theory take them to form a single constituent.Flat structure
The flat structure organizes small clause material into two distinct sister constituents.The a-trees on the left are the phrase structure trees, and the b-trees on the right are the dependency trees. The key aspect of these structures is that the small clause material consists of two separate sister constituents.
The flat analysis is preferred by those working in dependency grammars and representational phrase structure grammars.
Layered structure
The layered structure organizes small clause material into one constituent. The phrase structure trees are again on the left, and the dependency trees on the right. To mark the small clause in the phrase structure trees, the node label SC is used.The layered analysis is preferred by those working in the Government and Binding framework and its tradition, for examples see Chomsky, Ouhalla, Culicover, Haegeman and Guéron.
X-Bar Theory structure
See X-Bar Theory for a general exploration of X-Bar Theory.X-bar theory predicts that a head will project into an intermediate constituent and a maximal projection. There were three common analyses of the internal structure of a small clause under X-Bar theory. Here they are each presented as showing the NP AP small clause complement in the sentence, "I consider Mary smart":
The small clause as a symmetric constituent
In this analysis, neither of the constituents determine the category, meaning that it is an exocentric construction. Some linguists believe that the label of this structure can be symmetrically determined by the constituents, and others believe that this structure lacks a label altogether. In order to indicate a predicative relationship between the subject, and the predicate, some have suggested a system of co-indexation, where the subject must c-command any predicate associated with it.
This analysis is not compatible with X-bar theory because X-bar theory does not allow for headless constituents, additionally this structure may not be an accurate representation of a small clause because it lacks an intermediate functional element that connects the subject with the predicate. Evidence of this element can be seen as an overt realization in a variety of languages such as Welsh, Norwegian, and English, as in the examples below :
- I regard Fred as insane.
- I consider Fred as my best friend.
The small clause as a projection of the predicate
In this analysis, the small clause can be identified as a projection of the predicate. In this view, the specifier of the structure is the subject of the head. This analysis builds on Chomsky's model of phrase structure and is proposed by Stowell and Contreras.
The small clause as a projection of the functional category
The PrP category, was proposed for a few reasons, some of which are outlined below:
- This structure helps to account for coordination where the categories of the items being coordinated must be the same. This accounts for the mystery of phrases such as example 1 below, where in the bolded small clause an adjective phrase is being coordinated with a noun phrase, and it still has a grammatical reading. The addition of the PrP category helps to solve this issue by positing that the items being coordinated are under the intermediate projection of the Pr head, the Pr', as seen in example 2.
- # Mayor Shinn considered Eulalie talented and a tyrant.
- # Mayor Shinn considered Eulalie talented and a tyrant.
- This structure answers the question of the category of the word 'as' in small clause constructions such as "I regard Fred as my best friend". This structure was an issue because 'as' could only be analyzed as a prepositional phrase, but prepositions do not take adjective phrase complements. Analyzing 'as' as an overt realization of the Pr head, then the small clause structure under X-bar theory can be upheld.
- The prisoner seems/appears to be intelligent.
- The prisoner seems/appears intelligent.
- The prisoner seems/appears to leave every day at noon.
-
* The prisoner seems/appears leave every day at noon.
Examples
Indo-European
Germanic: English
The following sentences contain small clauses. The actual small clause is in bold in each example. In each of these sentences, the underlined expression functions as a predicate over the nominal immediately to its left.The verbs that license small clauses like these are a heterogeneous set, and include
- raising-to-object or ECM verbs like consider and want
- verbs like call and name, which subcategorize for an object NP and a predicative expression
- verbs like wipe and pound, which allow the appearance of a resultative predicate.
An important trait that all six examples above have in common is that the small clause lacks a verb. Indeed, this has been taken as a defining aspect of small clauses, i.e. to qualify as a small clause, a verb must be absent. If, however, one allows a small clause to contain a verb, then the following sentences can also be interpreted as containing small clauses:
The similarity across the sentences a-f and these four sentences g-j is obvious, since the same subject-predicate relationship is present in all ten sentences. Hence if one interprets sentences a-f as containing small clauses, one can also judge sentences g-j as containing small clauses. A defining characteristic of all ten of the small clauses in a-j is that the tense associated with finite clauses, which contain a finite verb, is absent.
Romance
French
French small clauses may appear in an NP AP, NP PP, and NP VP constructions. However, there are some restrictions on NP VP constructions.The following example is an NP AP small clause construction.
The following example is an NP PP small clause construction.
The following example is an NP VP small clause construction. The verb here is infinitival, without inflected tense, and takes a PP complement.
However, the following example is an NP VP small clause construction that is ungrammatical. Although the verb here is infinitival, it cannot grammatically take an AP complement.
Coordination tests in French do not provide consistent evidence for small clauses' constituency. Below is an example proving small clauses' constituency. The two small clauses in this example use an NP AP construction.
However, the example below makes an incorrect prediction about constituency.
Sportiche provides two possible interpretations of this data: either coordination is not a reliable constituency test or the current theory of constituency should be revised to include strings such as the ones predicted above.
Brazilian Portuguese
In Brazilian Portuguese, there are two types of small clauses: free small clauses and dependent small clauses.Dependent small clauses are similar to English in that they consist of an NP XP in a predicative relation. Like many other Romance languages, Brazilian Portuguese has free subject-predicate inversion, although it is restricted here to verbs with single arguments. Dependent small clauses may appear in either a standard, as in example, or an inverted form, as in example.
In contrast, free small clauses can only occur in the inverted form. The small clause construction in example has an XP NP construction, specifically an AP NP construction.
However, in example, using an NP AP formation renders the free small clause ungrammatical.
The classification of free small clauses is under debate. Some linguists argue that these free small clauses are actually cleft sentences with finite tense, while other linguists believe that free small clauses are tense phrases without inflected tense on the surface.
Balto-Slavic: Lithuanian
Lithuanian small clauses may occur in a NP NP or NP AP construction.NP PP constructions are not small clauses in Lithuanian as the PP does not enter into a predicative relationship with the NP.
The example below is of an NP NP construction.
The example below is of an NP AP construction. While the English translation of the sentence includes the auxiliary verb "was", it is not present in Lithuanian.
In Lithuanian, small clauses may be moved to the front of the sentence to become the topic. This suggests that the small clause operates as a single unit, or a constituent. Note that the sentence in example in English is ungrammatical so it is marked with an asterisk, but the sentence is grammatical in Lithuanian.
The phrase her an immature brat cannot be split up in example, which provides further evidence that the small clause behaves as a single unit.
Sinitic: Mandarin
In Mandarin, a small clause does not only lack a verb and tense, but also the presence of functional projections. The reason for this is that the lexical entries for particular nouns in Mandarin not only contain the categorical feature for nouns, but also for verbs. Thus even with the lack of functional projections, nominals can be predicative in a small clause.illustrates a complement small clause: it has no tense-marking, only a DP subject and an NP predicate. However, the semantic difference between Mandarin Chinese and English with regards to its small clauses are represented by example and. Though is the embedded small clause in the previous example, it cannot be a matrix clause. Despite having the same sentence structure, a small clause consisting of a DP and an NP, due to the ability of a nominal expression to also belong to a second category of verbs, example is a grammatical sentence. This is evidence that there are more restrictive constraints on what is considered a small clause in Mandarin Chinese, which requires further research.
Special case of small clause usage
Below is case of special usage of small clause used with the possessive verb yǒu. The small clause is underlined.Here, the possessive verb yǒu takes a small clause complement in order to make a degree comparison between the subject and indirect object. Due to the following AP gāo, here the possessive verb yǒu expresses a limit of the degree of tallness. It is only with a small clause complement that this uncommon degree use of the possessive verb can be communicated.
Korean
Below is an example of a fully complemented clause and an embedded small clause in Korean. The small clause is bolded.In the fully complemented clause, the sentence would be grammatical whether the object Mary is marked with a nominative case or an accusative case. However, as the subject of the small clause, the case marking for the object is strictly accusative. This demonstrates that raising-to-object is optional from a complete clauses and necessary from small clauses.
Literature
- Aarts, B. 1992. Small clauses in English: the non-verbal types. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
- Borsley, R. 1991. Syntactic theory: A unified approach. London: Edward Arnold.
- Chomsky, N. 1981. Lectures on government and binding: The Pisa lectures. Berlin:Mouton de Gruyter.
- Chomsky, N. 1986. Barriers. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
- Culicover, P. 1997. Principles and parameters: An introduction to syntactic theory. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
- Culicover, P. and R. Jackendoff. 2005. Simpler syntax. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
- Haegeman, L. 1994. Introduction to government and binding theory, 2nd edition. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
- Haegeman, L. and J. Guéron 1999. English grammar: A generative perspective. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishers.
- Matthews, P. 2007. Syntactic relations: A critical survey. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
- Ouhalla, J. 1994. Transformational grammar: From rules to principles and parameters. London: Edward Arnold.
- Wardhaugh, R. 2003. Understanding English grammar, second edition. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.