Japanese phonology


The phonology of Japanese features about 15 consonant phonemes, the cross-linguistically typical five-vowel system of, and a relatively simple phonotactic distribution of phonemes allowing few consonant clusters. It is traditionally described as having a mora as the unit of timing, with each mora taking up about the same length of time, so that the disyllabic may be analyzed as and dissected into four moras,,,, and.
Standard Japanese is a pitch-accent language, wherein the position or absence of a pitch drop may determine the meaning of a word: "chopsticks", "bridge", "edge".
Unless otherwise noted, the following describes the standard variety of Japanese based on the Tokyo dialect.

Consonants

See below for more in-detail descriptions of allophonic variation.
Non-coronal voiced stops between vowels may be weakened to fricatives, especially in fast or casual speech:
However, is further complicated by its variant realization as a velar nasal. Standard Japanese speakers can be categorized into 3 groups, which will be explained below. If a speaker pronounces a given word consistently with the allophone , that speaker will never have as an allophone in that same word. If a speaker varies between and or is generally consistent in using , then the velar fricative is always another possible allophone in fast speech.
may be weakened to nasal when it occurs within words—this includes not only between vowels but also between a vowel and a consonant. There is a fair amount of variation between speakers, however. suggests that the variation follows social class, while suggests that the variation follows age and geographic location. The generalized situation is as follows.
;At the beginning of words:
;In the middle of simple words :
In the middle of compound words morpheme-initially:
So, for some speakers the following two words are a minimal pair while for others they are homophonous:
To summarize using the example of hage はげ 'baldness':
Some phonologists posit a distinct phoneme, citing pairs such as 大硝子 'big sheet of glass' vs. 大烏 'big raven'.

Palatalization and affrication

The palatals and palatalize the consonants preceding them:
For coronal consonants, the palatalization goes further so that alveolo-palatal consonants correspond with dental or alveolar consonants :
and also palatalize to a palatal fricative : > hito
Of the allophones of, the affricate is most common, especially at the beginning of utterances and after, while fricative may occur between vowels. Both sounds, however, are in free variation.
In the case of the,, and, when followed by, historically, the consonants were palatalized with merging into a single pronunciation. In modern Japanese, these are arguably separate phonemes, at least for the portion of the population that pronounces them distinctly in English borrowings.
The vowel also affects consonants that it follows:
Although and occur before other vowels in loanwords, and are distinguished before vowels except . is still not distinguished from . Similarly, and usually do not occur even in loanwords so that English cinema becomes shinema シネマ; although they may be written スィ and ズィ respectively, they are rarely found even among the most innovative speakers and do not occur phonemically.

neutralization

The contrast between and is neutralized before and :. By convention, it is often assumed to be, though some analyze it as, the voiced counterpart to. The writing system preserves morphological distinctions, though spelling reform has eliminated historical distinctions except in cases where a mora is repeated once voiceless and once voiced, or where rendaku occurs in a compound word: つづく, いちづける from. Some dialects retain the distinctions between and and between and, while others retain only and but not and, or merge all four.

Moraic nasal

Some analyses of Japanese treat the moraic nasal as an archiphoneme ; other less abstract approaches take its uvular pronunciation as basic or treat it as coronal appearing in the syllable coda. In any case, it undergoes a variety of assimilatory processes. It is variously:
Studies in the 2010s have shown, however, that there is considerable variability in the realization of word-final, and that, possibly with a double or secondary articulation, is much more common than.
Some speakers produce before, pronouncing them as, while others produce a nasalized vowel before.
These assimilations occur beyond word boundaries.

Gemination

While Japanese features consonant gemination, there are some limitations in what can be geminated. Most saliently, voiced geminates are prohibited in native Japanese words. This can be seen with suffixation that would otherwise feature voiced geminates. For example, Japanese has a suffix, || that contains what calls a "floating mora" that triggers gemination in certain cases. When this would otherwise lead to a geminated voiced obstruent, a moraic nasal appears instead as a sort of "partial gemination".
In the late 20th century, voiced geminates began to appear in loanwords, though they are marked and have a high tendency to devoicing. A frequent example is loanwords from English such as bed and dog that, though they end with voiced singletons in English, are geminated when borrowed into Japanese. These geminates frequently undergo devoicing to become less marked, which gives rise to variability in voicing:
The distinction is not rigorous. For example, when voiced obstruent geminates appear with another voiced obstruent they can undergo optional devoicing. attributes this to a less reliable distinction between voiced and voiceless geminates compared to the same distinction in non-geminated consonants, noting that speakers may have difficulty distinguishing them due to the partial devoicing of voiced geminates and their resistance to the [|weakening process mentioned above], both of which can make them sound like voiceless geminates.
There is some dispute about how gemination fits with Japanese [|phonotactics]. One analysis, particularly popular among Japanese scholars, posits a special "mora phoneme" , which corresponds to the sokuon. However, not all scholars agree that the use of this "moraic obstruent" is the best analysis. In those approaches that incorporate the moraic obstruent, it is said to completely assimilate to the following obstruent, resulting in a geminate consonant. The assimilated remains unreleased and thus the geminates are phonetically long consonants. does not occur before vowels or nasal consonants. This can be seen as an archiphoneme in that it has no underlying place or manner of articulation, and instead manifests as several phonetic realizations depending on context, for example:
Another analysis of Japanese dispenses with. In such an approach, the words above are phonemicized as shown below:
Gemination can of course also be transcribed with a length mark, but this notation obscures mora boundaries.

Sandhi

Various forms of sandhi exist; the Japanese term for sandhi generally is, while sandhi in Japanese specifically is called. Most commonly, a terminal on one morpheme results in or being added to the start of the next morpheme, as in, てん + おう > てんのう. In some cases, such as this example, the sound change is used in writing as well, and is considered the usual pronunciation. See :ja:連声 for further examples.

Vowels

FrontCentralBack
Close
Mid
Open

Except for, the short vowels are similar to their Spanish counterparts.
Vowels have a phonemic length contrast. Compare contrasting pairs of words like ojisan 'uncle' vs. ojiisan 'grandfather', or tsuki 'moon' vs. tsūki 'airflow'.
Some analyses make a distinction between a long vowel and a succession of two identical vowels, citing pairs such as 砂糖屋 satōya 'sugar shop' vs. 里親 satooya 'foster parent'. They are usually identical in normal speech, but when enunciated a distinction may be made with a pause or a glottal stop inserted between two identical vowels.
Within words and phrases, Japanese allows long sequences of phonetic vowels without intervening consonants, pronounced with hiatus, although the pitch accent and slight rhythm breaks help track the timing when the vowels are identical. Sequences of two vowels within a single word are extremely common, occurring at the end of many i-type adjectives, for example, and having three or more vowels in sequence within a word also occurs, as in aoi 'blue/green'. In phrases, sequences with multiple o sounds are most common, due to the direct object particle を 'wo' being realized as o and the honorific prefix お〜 'o', which can occur in sequence, and may follow a word itself terminating in an o sound; these may be dropped in rapid speech. A fairly common construction exhibiting these is 「〜をお送りします」 ... o o-okuri-shimasu 'humbly send...'. More extreme examples follow:

Devoicing

In many dialects, the close vowels and become voiceless when placed between two voiceless consonants or, unless accented, between a voiceless consonant and a pausa.
Generally, devoicing does not occur in a consecutive manner:
This devoicing is not restricted to only fast speech, though consecutive voicing may occur in fast speech.
To a lesser extent, may be devoiced with the further requirement that there be two or more adjacent moras containing the same phoneme:
The common sentence-ending copula desu and polite suffix masu are typically pronounced and.
Japanese speakers are usually not even aware of the difference of the voiced and devoiced pair. On the other hand, gender roles play a part in prolonging the terminal vowel: it is regarded as effeminate to prolong, particularly the terminal as in arimasu. Some nonstandard varieties of Japanese can be recognized by their hyper-devoicing, while in some Western dialects and some registers of formal speech, every vowel is voiced.

Nasalization

Japanese vowels are slightly nasalized when adjacent to nasals. Before the moraic nasal, vowels are heavily nasalized:

Glottal stop insertion

At the beginning and end of utterances, Japanese vowels may be preceded and followed by a glottal stop, respectively. This is demonstrated below with the following words :
When an utterance-final word is uttered with emphasis, this glottal stop is plainly audible, and is often indicated in the writing system with a small letter tsu called a sokuon. This is also found in interjections like あっ and えっ. These words are likely to be romanized as and.

Phonotactics

Japanese words have traditionally been analysed as composed of moras; a distinct concept from that of syllables. Each mora occupies one rhythmic unit, i.e. it is perceived to have the same time value. A mora may be "regular" consisting of just a vowel or a consonant and a vowel, or may be one of two "special" moras, and. A glide may precede the vowel in "regular" moras. Some analyses posit a third "special" mora,, the second part of a long vowel. In this table, the period represents a mora break, rather than the conventional syllable break.
is restricted from occurring word-initially, and is found only word-medially. Vowels may be long, and the voiceless consonants may be geminate. In the analysis with archiphonemes, geminate consonants are the realization of the sequences, and sequences of followed by a voiceless obstruent, though some words are written with geminate voiced obstruents. In the analysis without archiphonemes, geminate clusters are simply two identical consonants, one after the other.
In English, stressed syllables in a word are pronounced louder, longer, and with higher pitch, while unstressed syllables are relatively shorter in duration. Japanese is often considered a mora-timed language, as each mora tends to be of the same length, though not strictly: geminate consonants and moras with devoiced vowels may be shorter than other moras. Factors such as pitch have negligible influence on mora length.

Accent

Standard Japanese has a distinctive pitch accent system: a word can have one of its moras bearing an accent or not. An accented mora is pronounced with a relatively high tone and is followed by a drop in pitch. The various Japanese dialects have different accent patterns, and some exhibit more complex tonic systems.

Sound change

As an agglutinative language, Japanese has generally very regular pronunciation, with much simpler morphophonology than a fusional language would. Nevertheless, there are a number of prominent sound change phenomena, primarily in morpheme combination and in conjugation of verbs and adjectives. Phonemic changes are generally reflected in the spelling, while those that are not either indicate informal or dialectal speech which further simplify pronunciation.

Sandhi

Rendaku

In Japanese, sandhi is prominently exhibited in rendaku – consonant mutation of the initial consonant of a morpheme from unvoiced to voiced in some contexts when it occurs in the middle of a word. This phonetic difference is reflected in the spelling via the addition of dakuten, as in. In cases where this combines with the yotsugana mergers, notably and in standard Japanese, the resulting spelling is morphophonemic rather than purely phonemic.

Gemination

The other common sandhi in Japanese is conversion of つ or く, and ち or き, and rarely ふ or ひ as a trailing consonant to a geminate consonant when not word-final – orthographically, the sokuon っ, as this occurs most often with つ. So that
Some long vowels derive from an earlier combination of a vowel and fu. The f often causes gemination when it is joined with another word:
Most words exhibiting this change are Sino-Japanese words deriving from Middle Chinese morphemes ending in, or, which were borrowed on their own into Japanese with a prop vowel after them but in compounds as assimilated to the following consonant.

Renjō

Sandhi also occurs much less often in, where, most commonly, a terminal or on one morpheme results in or respectively being added to the start of a following morpheme beginning with a vowel or semivowel, as in. Examples:
;First syllable ending with :
;First syllable ending with from original :
;First syllable ending with :
Another prominent feature is, particularly historical sound changes.
In cases where this has occurred within a morpheme, the morpheme itself is still distinct but with a different sound, as in, which underwent two sound changes from earlier → → → .
However, certain forms are still recognizable as irregular morphology, particularly forms that occur in basic verb conjugation, as well as some compound words.

Verb conjugation

Polite adjective forms

The polite adjective forms exhibit a one-step or two-step sound change. Firstly, these use the continuative form,, which exhibits onbin, dropping the k as →. Secondly, the vowel may combine with the preceding vowel, according to historical sound changes; if the resulting new sound is palatalized, meaning, this combines with the preceding consonant, yielding a palatalized syllable.
This is most prominent in certain everyday terms that derive from an i-adjective ending in -ai changing to , which is because these terms are abbreviations of polite phrases ending in gozaimasu, sometimes with a polite o- prefix. The terms are also used in their full form, with notable examples being:
Other transforms of this type are found in polite speech, such as → and →.

''-hito''

The morpheme has changed to or, respectively, in a number of compounds. This in turn often combined with a historical vowel change, resulting in a pronunciation rather different from that of the components, as in . These include:
In some cases morphemes have effectively fused and will not be recognizable as being composed of two separate morphemes.